Today, let’s review Cambridge cultural historian Peter Burke’s definition of the polymath. In fact, remember that he had four definitions:
The passive polymaths, who read widely but make their
reputation in one discipline alone.
The limited polymaths, active in a small cluster of
neighboring disciplines.
The serial polymaths, whose interests gradually shifted from
one discipline to others.
The proper polymaths, who have continued to work in several
fields and to make serious contributions to all of them, keeping several balls
in the air at the same time rather than picking them up one by one.
Also remember that Burke lamented that polymathy “has been
diluted to refer to people who have mastered two or three disciplines.”
How should we work these definitions into our own?
The limited polymath is clearly out as a definition for our
modern polymath. Yesterday, I suggested that the learning of a microbiologist
and neurobiologist is not broad enough to be fully polymathic. That is a good
example of Burke’s limited polymath.
But what about his passive polymath and his serial polymath?
The biggest problem, it seems, with the passive polymath is
that it is very hard to determine that person’s polymathic qualities. How do
you figure out if that person has read widely enough to qualify as a true
polymath? I don’t know. If you have any ideas, please let me know.
But the serial polymath seems pretty legitimate to me. So
what if the person concentrated in one field and then moved on to a different
field? There has never been a rule that a polymath has to engage in all of
his/her fields of interest at the same time. Some undoubtedly have, but that’s
not a requirement, at least not to me.
And then of course, we have Burke’s proper polymath, which
is a pretty legitimate polymath.
So, when we come up with our definition, possibly even
tomorrow, we’ll want to factor in Burke’s concepts of the serial and proper
polymaths.
No comments:
Post a Comment